Studies in the psychology of sex, volume 4 (of 6) by Havelock Ellis. - HTML preview

PLEASE NOTE: This is an HTML preview only and some elements such as links or page numbers may be incorrect.
Download the book in PDF, ePub, Kindle for a complete version.

use their vocal powers chiefly, and sometimes exclusively, during the

breeding-season, adds that "it is a surprising fact that we have not as

yet any good evidence that these organs are used by male mammals to charm

the female."[114] From a very different standpoint, Féré, in studying the

pathology of the human sexual instinct in the light of a very full

knowledge of the available evidence, states that he knows of no detailed

observations showing the existence of any morbid sexual perversions based

on the sense of hearing, either in reference to the human voice or to

instrumental music.[115]

When, however, we consider that not only in the animals most nearly

related to man, but in man himself, the larynx and the voice undergo a

marked sexual differentiation at puberty, it is difficult not to believe

that this change has an influence on sexual selection and sexual

psychology. At puberty there is a slight hyperæmia of the larynx,

accompanied by rapid development alike of the larynx itself and of the

vocal cords, which become larger and thicker, while there is an associated

change in the voice, which deepens. All these changes are very slight in

girls, but very pronounced in boys, whose voices are said to "break" and

then become lower by at least an octave. The feminine larynx at puberty

only increases in the proportion of 5 to 7, but the masculine larynx in

the proportion of 5 to 10. The direct dependence of this change on the

general sexual development is shown not merely by its occurrence at

puberty, but by the fact that in eunuchs in whom the testicles have been

removed before puberty the voice retains its childlike qualities.[116]

As a matter of fact, I believe that we may attach a considerable degree of

importance to the voice and to music generally as a method of sexual

appeal. On this point I agree with Moll, who remarks that "the sense of

hearing here plays a considerable part, and the stimulation received

through the ears is much larger than is usually believed."[117] I am not,

however, inclined to think that this influence is considerable in its

action on men, although Mantegazza remarks, doubtless with a certain

truth, that "some women's voices cannot be heard with impunity." It is

true that the ancients deprecated the sexual or at all events the

effeminating influence of some kinds of music, but they seem to have

regarded it as sedative rather than stimulating; the kind of music they

approved of as martial and stimulating was the kind most likely to have

sexual effects in predisposed persons.

The Chinese and the Greeks have more especially insisted on the

ethical qualities of music and on its moralizing and demoralizing

effects. Some three thousand years ago, it is stated, a Chinese

emperor, believing that only they who understood music are

capable of governing, distributed administrative functions in

accordance with this belief. He acted entirely in accordance with

Chinese morality, the texts of Confucianism (see translations in

the "Sacred Books of the East Series") show clearly that music

and ceremony (or social ritual in a wide sense) are regarded as

the two main guiding influences of life--music as the internal

guide, ceremony as the external guide, the former being looked

upon as the more important.

Among the Greeks Menander said that to many people music is a

powerful stimulant to love. Plato, in the third book of the

_Republic_, discusses what kinds of music should be encouraged in

his ideal state. He does not clearly state that music is ever a

sexual stimulant, but he appears to associate plaintive music

(mixed Lydian and Hypolydian) with drunkenness, effeminacy, and

idleness and considers that such music is "useless even to women

that are to be virtuously given, not to say to men."

He only

admits two kinds of music: one violent and suited to war, the

other tranquil and suited to prayer or to persuasion. He sets out

the ethical qualities of music with a thoroughness which almost

approaches the great Chinese philosopher: "On these accounts we

attach such importance to a musical education, because rhythm and

harmony sink most deeply into the recesses of the soul, and take

most powerful hold of it, bringing gracefulness in their train,

and making a man graceful if he be rightly nurtured,

... leading

him to commend beautiful objects, and gladly receive them into

his soul, and feed upon them, and grow to be noble and good."

Plato is, however, by no means so consistent and thorough as the

Chinese moralist, for having thus asserted that it is the

influence of music which molds the soul into virtue, he proceeds

to destroy his position with the statement that "we shall never

become truly musical until we know the essential forms of

temperance and courage and liberality and munificence," thus

moving in a circle. It must be added that the Greek conception of

music was very comprehensive and included poetry.

Aristotle took a wider view of music than Plato and admitted a

greater variety of uses for it. He was less anxious to exclude

those uses which were not strictly ethical. He disapproved,

indeed, of the Phrygian harmony as the expression of Bacchic

excitement. He accepts, however, the function of music as a

katharsis of emotion, a notion which is said to have originated

with the Pythagoreans. (For a discussion of Aristotle's views on

music, see W.L. Newman, _The Politics of Aristotle_, vol. i, pp.

359-369.)

Athenæus, in his frequent allusions to music, attributes to it

many intellectual and emotional properties (e.g., Book XIV,

Chapter XXV) and in one place refers to "melodies inciting to

lawless indulgence" (Book XIII, Chapter LXXV).

We may gather from the _Priapeia_ (XXVI) that cymbals and

castanets were the special accompaniment in antiquity of wanton

songs and dances: "_cymbala, cum crotalis, pruriginis arma_."

The ancient belief in the moralizing influence of music has

survived into modern times mainly in a somewhat more scientific

form as a belief in its therapeutic effects in disordered nervous

and mental conditions. (This also is an ancient belief as

witnessed by the well-known example of David playing to Saul to

dispel his melancholia.) In 1729 an apothecary of Oakham, Richard

Broune, published a work entitled _Medicina Musica_, in which he

argued that music was beneficial in many maladies.

In more recent

days there have been various experiments and cases brought

forward showing its efficacy in special conditions.

An American physician (W.F. Hutchinson) has shown that anæsthesia

may be produced with accurately made tuning forks at certain

rates of vibration (summarized in the _British Medical Journal_,

June 4, 1898). Ferrand in a paper read before the Paris Academy

of Medicine in September, 1895, gives reasons for classing some

kinds of music as powerful antispasmodics with beneficial

therapeutic action. The case was subsequently reported of a child

in whom night-terrors were eased by calming music in a minor key.

The value of music in lunatic asylums is well recognized; see

e.g., Näcke, _Revue de Psychiatrie_, October, 1897.

Vaschide and

Vurpas (_Comptes Rendus de la Société de Biologie_, December 13,

1902) have recorded the case of a girl of 20, suffering from

mental confusion with excitation and central motor disequilibrium, whose muscular equilibrium was restored and

movements rendered more co-ordinated and adaptive under the

influence of music.

While there has been much extravagance in the ancient doctrine

concerning the effects of music, the real effects are still

considerable. Not only is this demonstrated by the experiments

already referred to (p. 118), indicating the efficacy of musical

sounds as physiological stimulants, but also by anatomical

considerations. The roots of the auditory nerves, McKendrick has

pointed out, are probably more widely distributed and have more

extensive connections than those of any other nerve.

The

intricate connections of these nerves are still only being

unraveled. This points to an explanation of how music penetrates

to the very roots of our being, influencing by associational

paths reflex mechanisms both cerebral and somatic, so that there

is scarcely a function of the body that may not be affected by

the rhythmical pulsations, melodic progressions, and harmonic

combinations of musical tones. (_Nature_, June 15, 1899, p. 164.)

Just as we are not entitled from the ancient belief in the influence of

music on morals or the modern beliefs in its therapeutic influence--even

though this has sometimes gone to the length of advocating its use in

impotence[118]--to argue that music has a marked influence in exciting the

specifically sexual instincts, neither are we entitled to find any similar

argument in the fact that music is frequently associated with the

love-feelings of youth. Men are often able to associate many of their

earliest ideas of love in boyhood with women singing or playing; but in

these cases it will always be found that the fascination was romantic and

sentimental, and not specifically erotic.[119] In adult life the music

which often seems to us to be most definitely sexual in its appeal (such

as much of Wagner's _Tristan_) really produces this effect in part from

the association with the story, and in part from the intellectual

realization of the composer's effort to translate passion into æsthetic

terms; the actual effect of the music is not sexual, and it can well be

believed that the results of experiments as regards the sexual influence

of the _Tristan_ music on men under the influence of hypnotism have been,

as reported, negative. Helmholtz goes so far as to state that the

expression of sexual longing in music is identical with that of religious

longing. It is quite true, again, that a soft and gentle voice seems to

every normal man as to Lear "an excellent thing in woman," and that a

harsh or shrill voice may seem to deaden or even destroy altogether the

attraction of a beautiful face. But the voice is not usually in itself an

adequate or powerful method of evoking sexual emotion in a man. Even in

its supreme vocal manifestations the sexual fascination exerted by a great

singer, though certainly considerable, cannot be compared with that

commonly exerted by the actress. Cases have, indeed, been

recorded--chiefly occurring, it is probable, in men of somewhat morbid

nervous disposition--in which sexual attraction was exerted chiefly

through the ear, or in which there was a special sexual sensibility to

particular inflections or accents.[120] Féré mentions the case of a young

man in hospital with acute arthritis who complained of painful erections

whenever he heard through the door the very agreeable voice of the young

woman (invisible to him) who superintended the linen.[121] But these

phenomena do not appear to be common, or, at all events, very pronounced.

So far as my own inquiries go, only a small proportion of men would

appear to experience definite sexual feelings on listening to music. And

the fact that in woman the voice is so slightly differentiated from that

of the child, as well as the very significant fact that among man's

immediate or even remote ancestors the female's voice can seldom have

served to attract the male, sufficiently account for the small part played

by the voice and by music as a sexual allurement working on men.[122]

It is otherwise with women. It may, indeed, be said at the outset that the

reasons which make it antecedently improbable that men should be sexually

attracted through hearing render it probable that women should be so

attracted. The change in the voice at puberty makes the deeper masculine

voice a characteristic secondary sexual attribute of man, while the fact

that among mammals generally it is the male that is most vocal--and that

chiefly, or even sometimes exclusively, at the rutting season--renders it

antecedently likely that among mammals generally, including the human

species, there is in the female an actual or latent susceptibility to the

sexual significance of the male voice,[123] a susceptibility which, under

the conditions of human civilization, may be transferred to music

generally. It is noteworthy that in novels written by women there is a

very frequent attentiveness to the qualities of the hero's voice and to

its emotional effects on the heroine.[124] We may also note the special

and peculiar personal enthusiasm aroused in women by popular musicians, a

more pronounced enthusiasm than is evoked in them by popular actors.

As an interesting example of the importance attached by women

novelists to the effects of the male voice I may refer to George

Eliot's _Mill on the Floss_, probably the most intimate and

personal of George Eliot's works. In Book VI of this novel the

influence of Stephen Guest (a somewhat commonplace young man)

over Maggie Tulliver is ascribed almost exclusively to the effect

of his base voice in singing. We are definitely told of Maggie

Tulliver's "sensibility to the supreme excitement of music."

Thus, on one occasion, "all her intentions were lost in the vague

state of emotion produced by the inspiring duet--

emotion that

seemed to make her at once strong and weak: strong for all

enjoyment, weak for all resistance. Poor Maggie! She looked very

beautiful when her soul was being played on in this way by the

inexorable power of sound. You might have seen the slightest

perceptible quivering through her whole frame as she leaned a

little forward, clasping her hands as if to steady herself; while

her eyes dilated and brightened into that wideopen, childish

expression of wondering delight, which always came back in her

happiest moments." George Eliot's novels contain many allusions

to the powerful emotional effects of music.

It is unnecessary to refer to Tolstoy's _Kreutzer Sonata_, in

which music is regarded as the Galeotto to bring lovers

together--"the connecting bond of music, the most refined lust of

the senses."

In primitive human courtship music very frequently plays a considerable

part, though not usually the sole part, being generally found as the

accompaniment of the song and the dance at erotic festivals.[125] The

Gilas, of New Mexico, among whom courtship consists in a prolonged

serenade day after day with the flute, furnish a somewhat exceptional

case. Savage women are evidently very attentive to music; Backhouse (as

quoted, by Ling Roth[126]) mentions how a woman belonging to the very

primitive and now extinct Tasmanian race, when shown a musical box,

listened "with intensity; her ears moved like those of a dog or horse, to

catch the sound."

I have found little evidence to show that music, except in occasional

cases, exerts even the slightest specifically sexual effect on men,

whether musical or unmusical. But I have ample evidence that it very

frequently exerts to a slight but definite extent such an influence on

women, even when quite normal. Judging from my own inquiries it would,

indeed, seem likely that the majority of normal educated women are liable

to experience some degree of definite sexual excitement from music; one

states that orchestral music generally tends to produce this effect;

another finds it chiefly from Wagner's music; another from military music,

etc. Others simply state--what, indeed, probably expresses the experience

of most persons of either sex--that it heightens one's mood. One lady

mentions that some of her friends, whose erotic feelings are aroused by

music, are especially affected in this way by the choral singing in Roman

Catholic churches.[127]

In the typical cases just mentioned, all fairly normal and healthy women,

the sexual effects of music though definite were usually quite slight. In

neuropathic subjects they may occasionally be more pronounced. Thus, a

medical correspondent has communicated to me the case of a married lady

with one child, a refined, very beautiful, but highly neurotic, woman,

married to a man with whom she has nothing in common.

Her tastes lie in

the direction of music; she is a splendid pianist, and her highly trained

voice would have made a fortune. She confesses to strong sexual feelings

and does not understand why intercourse never affords what she knows she

wants. But the hearing of beautiful music, or at times the excitement of

her own singing, will sometimes cause intense orgasm.

Vaschide and Vurpas, who emphasize the sexually stimulating

effects of music, only bring forward one case in any detail, and

it is doubtless significant that this case is a woman. "While

listening to a piece of music X changes expression, her eyes

become bright, the features are accentuated, a smile begins to

form, an expression of pleasure appears, the body becomes more

erect, there is a general muscular hypertonicity. X

tells us that

as she listens to the music she experiences sensations very like

those of normal intercourse. The difference chiefly concerns the

local genital apparatus, for there is no flow of vaginal mucus.

On the psychic side the resemblance is marked."

(Vaschide and

Vurpas, "Du Coefficient Sexual de l'Impulsion Musicale,"

_Archives de Neurologie_, May, 1904.) It is sometimes said, or implied, that a woman (or a man) sings

better under the influence of sexual emotion. The writer of an

article already quoted, on "Woman in her Psychological Relations"

(_Journal of Psychological Medicine_, 1851), mentions that "a

young lady remarkable for her musical and poetical talents

naïvely remarked to a friend who complimented her upon her

singing: 'I never sing half so well as when I've had a

love-fit.'" And George Eliot says. "There is no feeling, perhaps,

except the extremes of fear and grief, that does not make a man

sing or play the better." While, however, it may be admitted that

some degree of general emotional exaltation may exercise a

favorable influence on the singing voice, it is difficult to

believe that definite physical excitement at or immediately

before the exercise of the voice can, as a rule, have anything

but a deleterious effect on its quality. It is recognized that

tenors (whose voices resemble those of women more than basses,

who are not called upon to be so careful in this respect) should

observe rules of sexual hygiene; and menstruation frequently has

a definite influence in impairing the voice (H.

Ellis, _Man and

Woman_, fourth edition, p. 290). As the neighborhood of

menstruation is also the period when sexual excitement is most

likely to be felt, we have here a further indication that sexual

emotion is not favorable to singing. I agree with the remarks of

a correspondent, a musical amateur, who writes:

"Sexual

excitement and good singing do not appear to be correlated. A

woman's emotional capacity in singing or acting may be remotely

associated with hysterical neuroses, but is better evinced for

art purposes in the absence of disturbing sexual influences. A

woman may, indeed, fancy herself the heroine of a wanton romance

and 'let herself go' a little in singing with improved results.

But a memory of sexual ardors will help no woman to make the best

of her voice in training. Some women can only sing their best

when they think of the other women they are outsinging. One girl

'lets her soul go out into her voice' thinking of jamroll,

another thinking of her lover (when she has none), and most, no

doubt, when they think of nothing. But no woman is likely to

'find herself' in an artistic sense because she has lost herself

in another sense--not even if she has done so quite respectably."

The reality of the association between the sexual impulse and music--and,

indeed, art generally--is shown by the fact that the evolution of puberty

tends to be accompanied by a very marked interest in musical and other

kinds of art. Lancaster, in a study of this question among a large number

of young people (without reference to difference in sex, though they were

largely female), found that from 50 to 75 per cent of young people feel an

impulse to art about the period of puberty, lasting a few months, or at

most a year or two. It appears that 464 young people showed an increased

and passionate love for music, against only 102 who experienced no change

in this respect. The curve culminates at the age of 15

and falls rapidly

after 16. Many of these cases were really quite unmusical.[128]

FOOTNOTES:

[86] This view has been more especially developed by J.B. Miner, _Motor,

Visual, and Applied Rhythms_, Psychological Review Monograph Supplements,

vol. v, No. 4, 1903.

[87] Sir S. Wilks, _Medical Magazine_, January, 1894; cf. Clifford

Allbutt, "Music, Rhythm, and Muscle," _Nature_, February 8, 1894.

[88] Bücher, _Arbeit und Rhythmus_, third edition, 1902; Wundt,

_Völkerpsychologie_, 1900, Part I, p. 265.

[89] Féré deals fully with the question in his book, _Travail et Plaisir_,

1904, Chapter III, "Influence du Rhythme sur le Travail."

[90] Fillmore, "Primitive Scales and Rhythms,"

_Proceedings of the

International Congress of Anthropology_, Chicago, 1893.

[91] "Love Songs among the Omaha Indians," in _Proceedings_ of same

congress.

[92] Groos, _Spiele der Menschen_, p. 33.

[93] "Analysis of the Sexual Impulse," _Studies in the Psychology of Sex_,

vol. iii.

[94] Féré, _Sensation et Mouvement_, Chapter V; id., _Travail et Plaisir_,

Chapter XII.

[95] Scripture, _Thinking, Feeling, Doing_, p. 85.

[96] Tarchanoff, "Influence de la Musique sur l'Homme et sur les Animaux,"

_Atti dell' XI Congresso Medico Internationale_, Rome, 1894, vol. ii, p.

153; also in _Archives Italiennes de Biologie_, 1894.

[97] "Love and Pain," _Studies in the Psychology of Sex_, vol. iii.

[98] Féré, _Travail et Plaisir_, Chapter XII, "Action Physiologique des

Sens Musicaux." "A practical treatise on harmony,"

Goblot remarks (_Revue

Philosophique_, July, 1901, p. 61), "ought to tell us in what way such an

interval, or such a succession of intervals, affects us.

A theoretical

treatise on harmony ought to tell us the explanation of these impressions.

In a word, musical harmony is a psychological science."

He adds that this

science is very far from being constituted yet; we have hardly even

obtained a glimpse of it.

[99] _American Journal of Psychology_, April, 1898.

[100] _American Journal of Psychology_, November, 1887.

The influence of

rhythm on the involuntary muscular system is indicated by the occasional

effect of music in producing a tendency to contraction of the bladder.

[101] _Archiv für Anatomie und Physiologie_

(Physiologisches Abtheilung),

1880, p